Examining the Intersection Between Instructional Design Decision-Making and Problem-Solving of Ill-Structured Problems through Bounded Rationality

DOI:10.59668/534.11934
Instructional DesignDecision-makingIll-Structured Problem-SolvingBounded Rationality
The theory of bounded rationality suggests that individuals engage in solving problems by making decisions by relying on their own cognitive abilities and limitations, information available to them at the time of deciding, and time constraints associated with making a decision. In this paper, I will argue that bounded rationality provides a necessary theoretical construct to further examine the intersection between decision-making and problem-solving of ill-structured problems in the field of learning, design, and technology.

Watch on YouTube

The process of decision-making requires an individual to make “a commitment to a course of action that is intended to yield result that are satisfying for specific individuals” (Yates, 2003, p. 24). To make a choice that meets optimization of a situation, individuals must gather information, identify possible solutions, weigh the advantages and disadvantages of each possibility, and move forward with a resolution (Skyttner, 2001). Decisions can be classified as rational or dynamic. Rational decisions typically allow for more time for individuals to identify problems, establish criteria, search for solutions, consider advantages and disadvantages, generate alternatives, and arrive at a solution (Klein, 2008). Dynamic decisions are often associated with time constraints, requiring an individual to make decisions quickly and with information available to them at a given time (Jonassen, 2010; Klein, 1998). 

Instructional designers often find themselves engaging in designing solutions for ill-structured problems. These problems vary in structure, complexity, dynamicity, and abstractness (Jonassen, 2010). Ill-structured problems are dynamic in that the situation and its associated contextual factors are not static; they change over time (Jonassen, 1997, 2008). Ill-structured problems are often complex and present several issues that can be addressed by a variety of different solutions. Complex problems that are time-sensitive warrant dynamic decision-making where instructional designers need to make effective decisions efficiently. 

The theory of bounded rationality was first introduced by Herbert Simon in 1955 as an alternative method to mathematical and economic modelling of decision-making.  During this time, the mathematical and economic models that were used to guide decisions in economics and political science assumed that all individuals were rational agents engaging in decision-making to maximize utility (Simon, 1955, 1957). A fundamental limitation to the pre-existing models is that they assumed perfect rationality. Simon argued that human beings are unable to approach decision-making practices with perfect rationality; rather, they operate within a bounded rationality. 

In this paper, I will argue that Simon’s (1955) bounded rationality provides a necessary theoretical construct to further examine the intersection between decision-making and problem-solving of ill-structured problems in the field of learning, design, and technology. I will provide examples of how the theory of bounded rationality can be applied by researchers in the field. Lastly, I will advocate for how bounded rationality, as a theoretical lens, can help forward the vision and mission of AECT. 

Bounded rationality suggests that individuals engage in solving problems by making decisions relying on their own cognitive abilities and limitations, information available to them at the time of deciding, and time constraints associated with making a decision (Simon, 1955, 1969). Bounded rationality, a rationality that is consistent with our knowledge of actual human choice behavior, assumes that the decision maker must search for alternatives, has egregiously incomplete and innocent knowledge about the consequences of actions, and chooses actions that are expected to be satisfactory” (Simon, 1997, p. 17). 

The theory of bounded rationality is grounded in three premises:

1. Human decisions should not be assumed a priori to follow logical, statistical or any other formal model;
2. There are three factors to take into account in decision making: the type of task, the characteristics of the environment, and the distinct features of the cognitive system that make the decision;
3. Only in conjunction with the collection of empirical data should formal computational models of decision-making processes be developed, and their predictions should be compared with human behavior.

    (Campitelli & Gobet, 2010, p. 355).

    Ill-structured problems prevalent in instructional design include, but are not limited to, addressing the existence of social inequities when designing instruction; designing within budgetary and time constraints to meet project deadlines, fostering interaction and engagement amongst learners in different instructional settings, and promoting inclusive design that makes learning accessible, affordable, and sustainable.  

    Scholars have recognized that learning, design, and technology projects are complex and ill-structured, often requiring instructional designers and researchers to engage in problem-solving and decision-making activities amidst ambiguity and uncertainty (Jonassen, 2012; Warr et al., 2020). Instructional designers engage in design conjecture to make decisions based on constrained information (Murty, 2010; Stefaniak et al., 2018; Stefaniak et al., 2022). 

    The premises of bounded rationality can support scholars in the field of learning, design, and technology by helping them to establish parameters around their design space in order to engage in dynamic decision-making practices (Stefaniak & Xu, 2020a; Stefaniak et al., 2021). By establishing parameters to frame design space, instructional designers can explore solutions that meet particular criteria (Dorst, 2019). 

    Proponents of bounded rationality recognize this as a viable construct because it recognizes and accounts for the imperfections of human decisions. The argument that human decisions should not be assumed a priori to follow a model contributes to the growing criticism in our field that that there are significant limitations to our current instructional design models (Gibbons, 2014; Moore, 2021; Stefaniak & Xu, 2020b). 

    Current instructional design models fail to address the systemic implications of design decisions (Kowch 2019; Nelson, 2020; Stefaniak & Xu, 2020b). Instructional design models are limiting in that they do not provide the necessary structure to address the dynamicity present in most design situations. If the goals of instructional design are to design solutions that are effective, efficient, and provide sustainable mean to support learning, instructional design scholars must attend to the systemic implications of their design decisions.

    Approaching instructional design through bounded rationality empowers instructional designers to accept the realities of situated contexts associated with their designs, establish parameters, and give themselves permission to focus on satisficing design problems rather than seeking perfection. It provides a mechanism to support dynamic decision-making to identify approach solutions to satisfy ill-structured problems. 

    Recognizing bounded rationality as a theory relevant to the field of learning, design, and technology will further support AECT’s vision to be the premier international organization in educational technology that others refer to for research and best practices. This theory can be leveraged to assist researchers and scholars with realistically considering the systemic implications of their scholarly activities. It enables them to establish parameters around their design space and consider contextual factors and stakeholders that will significantly influence their project outcomes. 

    Designing instructional interventions within a bounded rationality also helps instructional designers create viable and sustainable solutions that are an accurate and realistic reflection of situated contexts at any given time. By prioritizing this theory over others, scholarly organizations such as AECT can cultivate the next generation of instructional designers by providing them with guidance on how designing within bounded rationality can further support scholars who engage in decision-making and problem-solving activities for ill-structured problems in the field. It provides a mechanism to understand the realities that instructional design scholars and professionals face on a regular basis when engaging in ill-defined design projects.

    References

    Campitelli, G., & Gobet, F. (2010). Herbert Simon's decision-making approach: Investigation of cognitive processes in experts. Review of General Psychology14(4), 354-364. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021256 

    Dorst, K. (2019). Co-evolution and emergence in design. Design Studies, 65, 60-77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2019.10.005

    Gibbons, A. S. (2014). Eight views of instructional design and what they should mean to instructional designers. In B. Hokanson & A. S. Gibbons (Eds.), Design in educational technology (pp. 15-36). Springer. 

    Jonassen, D. H. (1997). Instructional design models for well-structured and ill-structured problem-solving learning outcomes. Educational Technology Research and Development45(1), 65-94. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02299613 

    Jonassen, D. H. (2008). Instructional design as design problem solving: An iterative process. Educational Technology, 48(3), 21-26.  https://www.jstor.org/stable/44429574

    Jonassen, D. H. (2010). Learning to solve problems: A handbook for designing problem-solving learning environments. Routledge. 

    Jonassen, D. H. (2012). Designing for decision making. Educational Technology Research and Development60(2), 341-359. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-011-9230-5

    Klein, G. A. (1998). Sources of power: How people make decisions. MIT Press. 

    Klein, G. (2008). Naturalistic decision making. Human Factors50(3), 456-460. https://doi.org/10.1518%2F001872008X288385

    Kowch, E. G. (2019). Introduction to systems thinking and change. In M.J. Spector, B.B. Lockee, & M.D. Childress (Eds.), Learning, design, and technology: An international compendium of theory, research, practice, and policy (pp. 1-14). Springer. 

    Murty, P., Paulini, M., & Maher, M. L. (2010). Collective intelligence and design thinking. In DRTS’10: Design Thinking Research Symposium (pp. 309-315). 

    Moore, S. (2021). The design models we have are not the design models we need. Journal of Applied Instructional Design, 10(4). https://dx.doi.org/10.51869/104/smo

    Nelson, H. G. (2020). The promise of systemic designing: Giving form to water. In M. J. Spector, B. B. Lockee, & M. D. Childress (Eds.), Learning, design, and technology: An international compendium of theory, research, practice, and policy (pp. 1-49). Springer. 

    Simon, H. A. (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 59, 99-118.

    Simon, H. A. (1957). Models of man, social and rationale: Mathematical essays on human behavior. Wiley.

    Simon, H. A. (1969). The sciences of the artificial. The M.I.T. Press.

    Simon, H. A. (1997). Models of bounded rationality: Empirically grounded economic reason. MIT Press. 

    Skyttner, L. (2001). General systems theory Ideas and applications. World Scientific Publishing. 

    Stefaniak, J., Baaki, J., Hoard, B., & Stapleton, L. (2018). The influence of perceived constraints during needs assessment on design conjecture. Journal of Computing in Higher Education30(1), 55-71. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12528-018-9173-5

    Stefaniak, J., Baaki, J., & Stapleton, L. (2022). An exploration of conjecture strategies used by instructional design students to support design decision-making. Educational Technology Research and Development, 1-29. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-022-10092-1

    Stefaniak, J., Luo, T., & Xu, M. (2021). Fostering pedagogical reasoning and dynamic decision-making practices: A conceptual framework to support learning design in a digital age. Educational Technology Research and Development69(4), 2225-2241. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-021-09964-9 

    Stefaniak, J., & Xu, M. (2020a). Leveraging dynamic decision-making and environmental analysis to support authentic learning experiences in digital environments. Revista De Educación a Distancia (RED)20(64). https://doi.org/10.6018/red.412171 

    Stefaniak, J., & Xu, M. (2020b). An examination of the systemic reach of instructional design models: A systematic review. TechTrends64(5), 710-719. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11528-020-00539-8

    Warr, M., Mishra, P., & Scragg, B. (2020). Designing theory. Educational Technology Research and Development68(2), 601-632. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-020-09746-9 

    Yates, J. F. (2003). Decision management. Jossey-Bass. 

    Jill E. Stefaniak

    University of Georgia

    Jill Stefaniak is an Associate Professor in the Learning, Design, and Technology program in the Department of Workforce Education and Instructional Technology at the University of Georgia. Her research interests focus on the professional development of instructional designers and design conjecture, designer decision-making processes, and contextual factors influencing design in situated environments.

    This content is provided to you freely by EdTech Books.

    Access it online or download it at https://edtechbooks.org/theory_comp_2022/bounded_rationality.